What is Action Bias?

Published
action bias in sports - a soccer goaltender jumps for the ball

Action bias is a cognitive bias characterized by the tendency of individuals to take action, even when it is not the optimal solution. This psychological phenomenon is rooted in the impulse to act, often driven by the discomfort of uncertainty or the desire for control.

In decision-making contexts, people may favor action over inaction, as doing something — anything — provides immediate feedback and the illusion of control. Its opposite effect is the omission bias.

People equate action with progress, which is not always accurate. In reality, a strategic pause or deliberation can sometimes yield better results than hurried actions. Recognizing and managing this bias involves self-awareness and critically evaluating when to act and when to wait, aligning actions with goals rather than reflexes.

Causes of Action Bias

Numerous hypotheses have been proposed to explain why individuals favor action over inaction. Humans may inherently be inclined to engage in behavior that they perceive as most advantageous, despite the fact that such behavior may occasionally result in an unfavorable consequence.

The lack of action may be regarded as a substandard substitute for participation. This viewpoint can be rationalized through an evolutionary lens, given that early behavior demonstrated its utility in ensuring survival, thereby solidifying its position as a reinforced behavioral pattern.

This bias endures in modern society because actions produce visible positive outcomes more so than inaction, a link that is reinforced, even though people’s living circumstances have evolved beyond the need to prefer action over inaction to ensure survival.

Cognitive Influences

Cognitive biases play a fundamental role in action bias. People often feel compelled to act due to the omission bias, where they perceive the costs of inaction to be greater than the costs of action.

Additionally, confirmation bias may cause individuals to favor actions that align with their pre-existing beliefs or hypotheses. For instance, a person may take action based on selective information that confirms their expectations, disregarding contrary evidence.

Emotional Factors

Emotional responses can significantly fuel action bias. Overconfidence in one’s ability to influence outcomes can lead to unnecessary actions, as individuals may believe they have more control over a situation than they actually do.

The discomfort of uncertainty can also provoke action as people may prefer to take any action rather than cope with the stress of the unknown. Conversely, staying within one’s comfort zone can lead to inaction, but the fear of future regret for what might have been can push individuals toward taking hasty steps.

Reinforcements are more effective than punishments in reducing the probability that an individual will exhibit a particular behavior, as demonstrated by operant conditioning. Individuals consequently opt for action as opposed to passivity. Additionally, taking action can function as a method of signalling and highlighting one’s efficiency to others, which is evaluated more favourably by society than favourable outcomes that result from remaining idle.

Action Bias in Different Contexts

In the business realm, a leadership principle often celebrated is the ‘bias for action,’ encouraging quick decision-making and initiative-taking. Companies like Amazon epitomize this approach, empowering teams to be productive and respond swiftly to market changes. It’s expected that business leaders and entrepreneurs show decisiveness, even when such actions involve substantial risks.

Environmental Policy

An investigation into the impact of action bias on environmental policy decisions was conducted by Anthony Patt and Richard Zeckhauser. They contended that action bias has a higher propensity to result in irrational decision-making within this domain as a consequence of factors such as the delayed impact and uncertainty of actions, contributions from numerous parties, the absence of efficient markets, ambiguous objectives, and weak incentives.

The research findings indicate that the perceived level of involvement, individual susceptibility to action bias, framing, and context all impact the value of a decision. These factors collectively contribute to the manifestation of action bias in environmental policies.

Medicine and Healthcare

Action bias can manifest in the domain of medicine during the process of diagnosis and subsequent treatment. This issue can be attributed, in part, to the utilization of particular diagnostic criteria.

An incorrect or premature diagnosis may result if a patient fails to satisfy an adequate number of criteria or meets precisely enough criteria. Consequently, the patient does not receive the treatment that is required or satisfactory.

Before reaching a conclusive diagnosis, one way to combat the action bias is to utilize a wider variety of tests or to seek a second opinion from colleagues and technical experts in the relevant disciplines. Medical decision-makers often exhibit a propensity for interference, despite the fact that refraining from doing so would be preferable. In this context, the action bias is referred to as intervention bias, and its existence has been substantiated by numerous medical studies.

Action bias is also present among patients. When presented with an equal choice by a physician between resting or taking medication, the majority of patients strongly prefer to take the medication. This inclination remains prevalent despite patients being duly informed of the potential adverse effects of the medication or explicitly informed that its use would have no discernible impact.

Sports and Performance Psychology

Action bias is noticeable in sports, such as soccer goalkeepers during penalty kicks; there’s a tendency to jump to one side, even though statistically, staying in the center can be more effective.  If you guess incorrectly, you almost certainly give up a goal, so it is more effective to stay still or wait to see which way the ball is kicked before moving.

The inclination to act may be driven by the anticipation of regret or a desire to visibly respond to a challenge, rather than the best chance of success.  Prior outcomes have an impact on action bias as well. In the event of a team’s defeat in a match, the coach is inclined towards selecting action over inaction by substituting certain players, despite the fact that this may not invariably result in an improved performance.

Addressing Action Bias

To enhance decision-making processes, individuals should focus on logic and analysis rather than quick, uninformed choices. They can exercise calculated risks by weighing the potential benefits against the adverse outcomes.

It’s critical to step out of one’s comfort zone while maintaining an acceptable risk tolerance. This balance promotes thoughtful action that is neither rushed nor a product of overthinking.

Complete disclosure regarding the consequences of actions, particularly adverse drug side effects, and abstention during treatment can mitigate the impact of the action bias in medical settings. A further ten percent of individuals opt for medication when their physician actively discourages its use.

Having awareness of the action bias can assist one in considering the repercussions of inaction versus action in a given circumstance. This results in a less impulsive process that incorporates logical reasoning, which aids in selecting the most effective course of action.

References:
  1. Bar-Eli, Michael; Azar, Ofer H.; Ritov, Ilana; Keidar-Levin, Yael; Schein, Galit (2007-10-01). Action bias among elite soccer goalkeepers: The case of penalty kicks. Journal of Economic Psychology. 28 (5): 606–621. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2006.12.001
  2. Kiderman, Alexander; Ilan, Uri; Gur, Itzhak; Bdolah-Abram, Tali; Brezis, Mayer (August 2013). Unexplained complaints in primary care: evidence of action bias. The Journal of Family Practice. 62 (8): 408–413
  3. Mercer, Kenneth L. (March 2019). Action Bias. Journal AWWA. 111 (3): 2. doi: 10.1002/awwa.1244
  4. Patt, Anthony; Zeckhauser, Richard (2000-07-01). Action Bias and Environmental Decisions. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 21 (1): 45–72. doi: 10.1023/A:1026517309871
  5. Thorpe, Alistair; Sirota, Miroslav; Juanchich, Marie; Orbell, Sheina (September 2020). Action bias in the public’s clinically inappropriate expectations for antibiotics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 26 (3): 422–431. doi: 10.1037/xap0000269
  6. Zeelenberg, Marcel; van den Bos, Kees; van Dijk, Eric; Pieters, Rik (2002). The inaction effect in the psychology of regret. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 82 (3): 314–327. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.3.314