Inspite of its rising popularity, using a weak electric current to boost brainpower does not live up to the hype. A new study demonstrates that the most common form of the treatment actually has a statistically significant detrimental effect on IQ scores.
The study adds to the increasing amount of literature showing that transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has mixed results when it comes to cognitive enhancement.
Senior author Flavio Frohlich, assistant professor of psychiatry, cell biology and physiology, biomedical engineering, and neurology at the University of North Carolina School of Medicine, says:
“It would be wonderful if we could use tDCS to enhance cognition because then we could potentially use it to treat cognitive impairment in psychiatric illnesses.
So, this study is bad news. Yet, the finding makes sense. It means that some of the most sophisticated things the brain can do, in terms of cognition, can’t necessarily be altered with just a constant electric current.”
Brain Stimulation Types
Frohlich does say that using a less common alternating current stimulation, so-called tACS, could be a better approach, one that he has been investigating.
Earlier this year, Frohlich’s lab found that tACS significantly boosted creativity, likely because he used it to target the brain’s natural electrical alpha oscillations, which have been implicated in creative thought.
With tDCS, scientists don’t target these brain waves, which represent neuronal patterns of communication throughout regions of the brain. Instead, they use tDCS to target brain structures, such particular regions of the cortex.
“All of our brain structures look more or less the same, but the reason why we’re all so different is that the electrical brain activities in our brains are very different,” Frohlich says. “We have to better understand this and target specific brain activity patterns.”
Study Explosion
The idea of using a weak electrical current to boost the brain’s natural abilities has existed for over a hundred years.
In 1801, Giovanni Aldini started a study in which he successfully used the technique of direct current stimulation to improve the mood of melancholy patients.
There was a brief rise of interest in transcranial direct current stimulation in the 1960s when studies by researcher D. J. Albert proved that the stimulation could affect brain function by changing the cortical excitability. He also discovered that positive and negative stimulation had different effects on the cortical excitability.
The current boom within the science community started in 2000, when German scientists published a paper showing that tDCS could alter the excitability of neurons in the motor cortex — the brain region that controls voluntary body movement.
Since then, there’s been an explosion of tDCS studies to try to make neurons more active or less active and therefore change outcomes for a variety of brain functions, such as working memory and cognitive acuity, and for illnesses, such as depression and schizophrenia.
But Frohlich, who is a member of the UNC Neuroscience Center, says that scientists still don’t know exactly what the direct current does to neural activity.
He also says some of the studies that have made waves were poorly designed. Some studies were not properly double-blinded or properly placebo controlled. Other studies were very small—fewer than 10 people.
A recent meta-analysis of a large number of tDCS papers showed that tDCS is far from a magic pill for cognitive enhancement or brain-related health conditions.
“Aside from stimulating the motor cortex, which has very exciting implications for stroke rehabilitation, I think the jury is still out on tDCS,” says Frohlich.
Electrodes On Their Minds
Frohlich’s team, including graduate student Kristin Sellers, the paper’s first author, recruited 40 healthy adults, each of whom took the standard WAIS-IV intelligence test. It is the most common and well-validated test of IQ, and includes tests for verbal comprehension, perceptional reasoning, working memory, and processing speed.
A week later, Frohlich’s team divided the participants into two groups.
Electrodes were placed on each side of each participant’s scalp, under which sat the frontal cortex. A third electrode, which sent electricity back to the device that that produced the electric current, was placed on top of the scalp.
Duke University collaborator and coauthor Angel Peterchev created imaging simulations to ensure Frohlich’s team targeted the same parts of the cortex that previous tDCS studies had targeted.
Then the placebo group received sham stimulation, a brief electrical current, which led participants to think they had received the full tDCS. The other participants received the standard tDCS for twenty minutes—a weak electrical current of 2 millioamperes.
All participants then retook the IQ tests. Frohlich expected that most, if not all, IQ scores would improve but that the participants who received tDCS would not improve their scores any more than would the people who did not undergo tDCS.
Sinking Scores
Frohlich’s team did find that all scores improved, most likely because of the “practice effect” of previously taking the test.
Surprisingly, the participants who did not receive tDCS saw their IQ scores increase by ten points, whereas participants who received tDCS saw their IQ scores increase by just shy of six points, on average.
When Frohlich and colleagues analyzed the test scores, they saw that the scores for three of the four kinds of cognitive tests were very similar between the two groups of participants. But the scores for perceptual reasoning were much lower among people who underwent tDCS.
Perceptual reasoning tests fluid intelligence, which is defined as the ability to think logically and apply innovative problem-solving to new problems.
Within the category of perceptual reasoning, the researchers saw the biggest differences in the subcategory of matrix reasoning—when participants viewed two groups of symbols and had to find the one symbol missing from the other group.
“This is one of the classical assays of fluid intelligence,” Frohlich says, “where you have to identify hidden rules and apply logic to find a missing element.”
The Importance of Research
Frohlich emphasizes:
“Our findings do not preclude the possibility that other tDCS paradigms may be less harmful or even beneficial. However it is time to make sure that everybody uses gold standard, placebo-controlled, double-blind study designs. Also, our study demonstrates the importance of more research on how stimulation interacts with brain activity.”
Frohlich cautions that the scientific community should be careful not to create simplistic storylines about tDCS being a “magic pill” for many brain-related conditions:
“There could be dangerous consequences, especially if tDCS is used daily. Ours was an acute study. We don’t know what the long-term effects are. There is so much more we need to understand before tDCS is ready for home use without medical supervision. I think our study demonstrates that we need to think of smarter ways to engage the brain to really target the specific brain dynamics involved in what we want to improve, such as cognition for people with depression or schizophrenia. I think tACS is an option, as well as more sophisticated modalities we’ve yet to develop.”
Reference:
- Kristin K. Sellers, Juliann M. Mellin, Caroline M. Lustenberger, Michael R. Boyle, Won Hee Lee, Angel V. Peterchev, Flavio Frohlich. Transcranial direct current stimulation of frontal cortex decreases performance on the WAIS-IV intelligence test. Behavioural Brain Research (2015) doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.031
Last Updated on November 14, 2023