Memory researchers at the University of Wisconsin have shone a light on how things get stored in the brain’s working memory, responsible for holding attention on small things of short-term importance.
Brad Postle, a psychology professor at the University of Wisconsin-Madison, explains:
“A lot of mental illness is associated with the inability to choose what to think about. What we’re taking are first steps toward looking at the mechanisms that give us control over what we think about."
Working Memory Ideas
Postle’s lab is investigating the concept that working memory remembers things through sustained brain activity. They caught brains tucking less-important information away somewhere beyond the reach of the tools that typically monitor brain activity.
And then they snapped that information back into active attention with magnets.
“The notion that you’re aware of everything all the time is a sort of illusion your consciousness creates,” says Postle. “That is true for thinking, too. You have the impression that you’re thinking of a lot of things at once, holding them all in your mind. But lots of research shows us you’re probably only actually attending to—are conscious of in any given moment—just a very small number of things."
It’s a bit like our vision, in which it feels like we’re seeing everything in our field of view, but details slip away unless you re-focus on them regularly.
Postle’s team conducted a series of experiments in which people were asked to remember two items representing different types of information (they used words, faces and directions of motion) because they’d be tested on their memories.
When the researchers gave their subjects a cue as to the type of question coming—a face, for example, instead of a word—the electrical activity and blood flow in the brain associated with the word memory disappeared. But if a second cue came letting the subject know they would now be asked about that word, the brain activity would jump back up to a level indicating it was the focus of attention.
The researchers were able to bring the seemingly abandoned items back to mind without cueing their subjects. Using a technique called transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to apply a focused electromagnetic field to a precise part of the brain involved in storing the word, they could trigger the sort of brain activity representative of focused attention.
Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation
“People have always thought neurons would have to keep firing to hold something in memory. Most models of the brain assume that. But we’re watching people remember things almost perfectly without showing any of the activity that would come with a neuron firing,
says Postle.
The fact that you’re able to bring it back at all in this example proves it’s not gone. It’s just that we can’t see evidence for its active retention in the brain."
Furthermore, if they cued their research subjects to focus on a face – causing brain activity associated with the word to drop off- a well-timed pulse of transcranial magnetic stimulation would snap the stowed memory back into attention, and prompt the subjects to incorrectly think that they had been cued to focus on the word.
“We think that memory is there, but not active,” says Postle. “More than just showing us it’s there, the TMS can actually make that memory temporarily active again."
The study, conducted by Postle with Nathan Rose, a former UW-Madison postdoctoral researcher who is now a professor of psychology at the University of Notre Dame, and UW-Madison graduate students in psychology and neuroscience, suggests a state of memory apart from the spotlight attention of active working memory and the deep storage of more significant things in long-term memory.
“What’s still unknown here is how the brain determines what falls away, and what enables you to retrieve things in the short-term if you need them,” Postle says.
Studying how the brain apportions attention could eventually influence the way we understand and treat mental health disorders such as schizophrenia, in which patients focus on hallucinations instead of reality, and depression, which seems strongly related to spending an unhealthy amount of time dwelling on negative things.
“We are making some interesting progress with very basic research,” says Postle. “But you can picture a point at which this work could help people control their attention, choose what they think about, and manage or overcome some very serious problems associated with a lack of control."
Nathan S. Rose, Joshua J. LaRocque, Adam C. Riggall, Olivia Gosseries, Michael J. Starrett, Emma E. Meyering, Bradley R. Postle
Reactivation of latent working memories with transcranial magnetic stimulation
Science 02 Dec 2016: Vol. 354, Issue 6316, pp. 1136-1139 DOI: 10.1126/science.aah7011